One of the key aims of public health is to assess the cause of disease or bad outcomes so we can design interventions.  In order to do that, we need to be able to tell the difference between when something is actually “causing” an outcome and when the exposure or condition is simply “associated” or “correlated” with an outcome. 

Whether something causes or is simply associated with a bad outcome is a key factor when we design interventions.  The following examples may shed some light on the relationship between risk factors, outcomes, and the difference between association and causation.

A study in the American Journal of Pathology “…persons with tattoos appear to die earlier than those without”.   The study found that people in the study group with a tattoo died 14 years earlier than people without a tattoo (p = .0001).

This study doesn’t conclude that having a tattoo actually causes people to die earlier.  Rather, it suggests that having a tattoo may be associated or correlated with other independent factors that might lead to an earlier death (e.g. people with tattoos may be more likely to have risk-taking behaviors).

In order to conclude that an exposure or condition actually causes an outcome, researchers randomly divide study participants into groups by assigning them to the exposure or condition they’re studying (experimental group) while making sure that another group doesn’t have the exposure or condition (control group). 

If the expected outcome is observed within an experimental group and not in the control group, it’s likely that exposure actually caused the outcome.

For example, if researchers were to expose one randomly-selected group of people to poison ivy via direct contact with poison ivy leaves – while not exposing the control group to poison ivy – they would most likely be able to establish that poison ivy actually caused the rash.  It’s this random assignment to conditions that make experiments sophisticated enough to detect actual causation.

Judging the causal significance of an association or causation is both a science and an art.  The gold standard for determining what is an association and what is actual causation is described in a 1964 Surgeon General’s Report on this topic.

Some of the research you read about shows a correlation or association between variables, not causation.  When you’re reading scientific studies, make sure you look for whether the study is talking about an “association or correlation” or whether they are talking about causation.

Editorial Note: Mr. Robert F Kennedy Jr appears to need an in-depth tutelage on the difference between anecdotes and stories and the difference between association & causation. Time and again Mr. Kennedy bases his opinions on spurious reports, anecdotal stories and some articles that draw conclusions about the association between various stimuli and outcomes rather than focusing on causation.

That mind-set is relatively benign when it’s held by a private sector lawyer with a famous name – but it’s potentially dangerous when the person is in charge of nearly 30% of the U.S. federal budget, health care for the majority of the country and comprehensive regulatory authority of drugs, food and medical devices, and the US publicly funded research mission.

RFK Jr. as HHS Secretary: His Shift Away from Evidence Puts Public Health in Peril – AZ Public Health Association

RFK Jr. as HHS Secretary? Why It Could Mean the End of Evidence-Based Public Health Policy – AZ Public Health Association